Germán Rodríguez
Generalized Linear Models Princeton University

Solutions to Problem Set 5:
Irish Educational Transitions

Statlib is an excellent source of statistical data and programs. Their Irish Educational Transitions dataset has data for 500 Irish school children aged 11 in 1967. The outcome of interest is educational attainment, which for purposes of this analysis I have recoded in educg in three categories: junior, senior, and 3rd level.

The predictors of interest are gender, a measure of father’s occupational prestige which I have recoded into quartiles in prestigeg, and the score in a reasoning test, which I also recoded into quartiles in reasong. The file irished.dta in the datasets section includes these recodes and drops primary school leavers and a few cases with missing data, for an effective sample size of 435.

. use https//grodri.github.io/datasets/irished, clear
(Irish Educational Transitions)

. desc

Contains data from d:/dataweb/wws509/datasets/irished.dta
 Observations:           435                  Irish Educational Transitions
    Variables:             9                  16 Nov 2022 16:06
────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
Variable      Storage   Display    Value
    name         type    format    label      Variable label
────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
gender          float   %9.0g      gender     
reason          float   %9.0g                 Score in Drumcondra verbal
                                                reasoning test
educ            float   %9.0g                 Educational level attained
cert            float   %9.0g      cert       Leaving certificate taken
prestige        float   %9.0g                 Prestige score for father's
                                                occupation
school          float   %23.0g     school     Type of school
educg           float   %9.0g      educg      Educational level attained
                                                (grouped)
prestigeg       float   %9.0g      prestigeg
                                              Prestige score for father's occ
                                                (grouped)
reasong         float   %9.0g      reasong    Score in Drymmcondra v.r. test
                                                (grouped)
────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
Sorted by: 
> library(haven)
> library(dplyr)
> irish <- read_dta("https://grodri.github.io/datasets/irished.dta") |>
+     mutate_if(is.labelled, as_factor) # labeled to factor
> head(irish)
# A tibble: 6 × 9
  gender reason  educ cert  prestige school     educg  prestigeg reasong
  <fct>   <dbl> <dbl> <fct>    <dbl> <fct>      <fct>  <fct>     <fct>  
1 male      113     3 no          28 secondary  junior Q1        Q4     
2 male      110     9 yes         69 secondary  senior Q4        Q3     
3 male      121     5 no          57 secondary  junior Q4        Q4     
4 male       82     4 no          18 vocational junior Q1        Q1     
5 male       85     4 no          28 vocational junior Q1        Q1     
6 male       98     2 no          43 vocational junior Q3        Q2     

[1] Multinomial Logits

(a) Fit a multinomial logit model explaining educational attaintment in terms of gender, parental occupational prestige, and scores in the reasoning test, using the recoded variables.

. mlogit educg i.gender i.prestigeg i.reasong, nolog

Multinomial logistic regression                         Number of obs =    435
                                                        LR chi2(14)   = 135.45
                                                        Prob > chi2   = 0.0000
Log likelihood = -365.44159                             Pseudo R2     = 0.1563

─────────────┬────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
       educg │ Coefficient  Std. err.      z    P>|z|     [95% conf. interval]
─────────────┼────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
junior       │  (base outcome)
─────────────┼────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
senior       │
      gender │
     female  │   .3051277   .2304158     1.32   0.185     -.146479    .7567344
             │
   prestigeg │
         Q2  │   .8704873   .3023455     2.88   0.004     .2779009    1.463074
         Q3  │   1.189704   .3376027     3.52   0.000     .5280148    1.851393
         Q4  │   1.340194   .3288149     4.08   0.000     .6957287    1.984659
             │
     reasong │
         Q2  │  -.0830386   .2990186    -0.28   0.781    -.6691042     .503027
         Q3  │   1.035168   .3063952     3.38   0.001     .4346448    1.635692
         Q4  │   1.627113   .3488457     4.66   0.000     .9433884    2.310838
             │
       _cons │   -1.65099   .3203233    -5.15   0.000    -2.278812   -1.023167
─────────────┼────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
3rd_level    │
      gender │
     female  │   .1615339   .3477705     0.46   0.642    -.5200838    .8431517
             │
   prestigeg │
         Q2  │     1.5331   .5534948     2.77   0.006     .4482699     2.61793
         Q3  │   1.682486   .5878532     2.86   0.004     .5303152    2.834657
         Q4  │   2.226992   .5433381     4.10   0.000     1.162069    3.291915
             │
     reasong │
         Q2  │   2.110641   1.078963     1.96   0.050    -.0040878    4.225369
         Q3  │   3.233068   1.064462     3.04   0.002     1.146761    5.319375
         Q4  │   4.963772   1.053335     4.71   0.000     2.899273     7.02827
             │
       _cons │  -5.793072   1.116641    -5.19   0.000    -7.981649   -3.604496
─────────────┴────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────

. scalar ml = e(ll)

We need multinom() in the nnet package.

> library(nnet)
> # make sure outcome is a factor or a matrix!
> irish$educg <- as.factor(irish$educg)
> ml <- multinom(educg ~ gender + prestigeg + reasong , data=irish)
# weights:  27 (16 variable)
initial  value 477.896346 
iter  10 value 369.535450
iter  20 value 365.464546
final  value 365.441586 
converged
> summary(ml)
Call:
multinom(formula = educg ~ gender + prestigeg + reasong, data = irish)

Coefficients:
          (Intercept) genderfemale prestigegQ2 prestigegQ3 prestigegQ4
senior      -1.650999    0.3051297   0.8704957    1.189714    1.340206
3rd level   -5.792979    0.1615402   1.5331076    1.682500    2.227006
            reasongQ2 reasongQ3 reasongQ4
senior    -0.08303942  1.035163  1.627145
3rd level  2.11053104  3.232968  4.963707

Std. Errors:
          (Intercept) genderfemale prestigegQ2 prestigegQ3 prestigegQ4
senior      0.3203241    0.2304163   0.3023462   0.3376034   0.3288158
3rd level   1.1165939    0.3477700   0.5534933   0.5878517   0.5433370
          reasongQ2 reasongQ3 reasongQ4
senior    0.2990188 0.3063954 0.3488479
3rd level 1.0789145 1.0644124 1.0532858

Residual Deviance: 730.8832 
AIC: 762.8832 

(b) Interpret the coefficient for females in both equations.

. di exp(_b[senior:2.gender]), exp(_b[3rd_level:2.gender])
1.3567983 1.1753123
> exp(coef(ml)[, "genderfemale"]) -1
   senior 3rd level 
0.3568010 0.1753197 

The relative probability of senior rather than junior level is 36% higher, and the relative probability of 3rd rather than junior level is 18% higher, for females than for males in the same quartile of parental occupational prestige and reasoning scores.

(c) Compute the average marginal effect of gender on the probability of achieving 3rd level, using the continuous approximation “by hand”.

We follow the calculations in the R and Stata logs. First we predict probabilities, then calculate the marginal effect, and finally average. I use equation numbers for brevity. I also calculate marginal effects for all 3 categories but only the last one was required.

. predict p1 p2 p3, pr

. gen sumpb = p2 * _b[2:2.gender] + p3 * _b[3:2.gender]

. gen me1 = p1 * (               - sumpb)

. gen me2 = p2 * (_b[2:2.gender] - sumpb)

. gen me3 = p3 * (_b[3:2.gender] - sumpb)

. sum me1 me2 me3

    Variable │        Obs        Mean    Std. dev.       Min        Max
─────────────┼─────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
         me1 │        435   -.0547583    .0164814  -.0751537  -.0197663
         me2 │        435    .0581615    .0107572   .0377484   .0749559
         me3 │        435   -.0034032    .0079186   -.022972   .0049748
> probs <- predict(ml, type = "probs")
> b <- c(0, coef(ml)[,"genderfemale"])
> pb <- probs[, 2] * b[2] + probs[, 3] * b[3]
> me <- matrix(0, nrow(probs), ncol(probs))
> for(j in 1:ncol(probs)) {
+   me[, j] <- probs[, j] * (b[j] - pb)
+ }
> apply(me, 2, mean)
[1] -0.054758571  0.058161404 -0.003402833

We find that the average probability of achieving third level is practically the same for females and comparable males, only 0.34 percentage points lower. (The other two marginal effects tell us that they have a lower probability of junior level, compensated almost exactly by a higher probability of senior level, namely -5.48 and 5.82 for a net difference of 0.34.)

(d) Reestimate the average marginal effect of gender on the probability of achieving 3rd level using the exact discrete calculation, also “by hand”.

To do this calculation we need to set gender to female and then to male, predict the probabilities each time, and then compute discrete differences and average

. gen keep = gender

. forvalues g = 1/2 {
  2.   quietly replace gender = `g'
  3.   predict p`g'1 p`g'2 p`g'3, pr
  4. }

. forvalues j=1/3 {
  2.   gen de`j' = p2`j' - p1`j'
  3. }

. sum de1 de2 de3

    Variable │        Obs        Mean    Std. dev.       Min        Max
─────────────┼─────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
         de1 │        435   -.0544882    .0163955   -.074616  -.0214939
         de2 │        435    .0579666    .0105146   .0418357   .0742811
         de3 │        435   -.0034784    .0080581  -.0213806   .0043799

. quietly replace gender = keep
> library(dplyr)
> males <- mutate(irish, gender = factor(1, levels=1:2, labels=c("male","female")))
> pmale <- predict(ml, type = "probs", newdata = males)
> females <- mutate(irish, gender = factor(2, levels=1:2, labels=c("male","female")))
> pfemale <- predict(ml, type = "probs", newdata = females)
> mpfemale <- apply(pfemale, 2, mean)
> mpmale   <- apply(pmale, 2, mean)
> data.frame(rbind(female = mpfemale, male = mpmale, diff = mpfemale - mpmale))
            junior     senior   X3rd.level
female  0.44118814 0.42318310  0.135628766
male    0.49567669 0.36521657  0.139106745
diff   -0.05448855 0.05796653 -0.003477979

The results are very similar to the continuous calculation. Girls have almost the same probability of achieving third level as comparable boys, on average 0.35 percentage points lower. (The other two discrete differences show a lower probability of junior level of 5.45 percentage points compensated by a higher probability of senior level of 5.80 points.)

(e) How would you go about testing the goodness of fit of this model considering that we have individual data? No need to do anything, just explain what you would do.

The deviance does not have a chi-squared distribution with individual data, so we need to create groups. Fortunately all predictors are categorical variables, with 32 possible combinations of gender and quartiles of occupational prestige and resoning score. We could group the data into these 32 groups, and then the deviance distribution should be well approximated by a chi-squared distribution, as the groups would average almost 14 observations each.

[2] Sequential Logits

(a) Fit a sequential logit model using the continuation ratio method, where you first focus on the probability of going beyond the junior level, and then look at the conditional probability of achieving 3rd form among those going beyond the junior level.

. gen beyond = educg > 1

. logit beyond i.gender i.prestigeg i.reasong, nolog

Logistic regression                                     Number of obs =    435
                                                        LR chi2(7)    = 100.36
                                                        Prob > chi2   = 0.0000
Log likelihood = -250.36968                             Pseudo R2     = 0.1670

─────────────┬────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
      beyond │ Coefficient  Std. err.      z    P>|z|     [95% conf. interval]
─────────────┼────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
      gender │
     female  │   .2836095   .2231757     1.27   0.204    -.1538069    .7210259
             │
   prestigeg │
         Q2  │   .9807345   .2927419     3.35   0.001      .406971    1.554498
         Q3  │   1.268714   .3279591     3.87   0.000     .6259258    1.911502
         Q4  │   1.503489   .3171323     4.74   0.000     .8819216    2.125057
             │
     reasong │
         Q2  │   .1002454   .2892327     0.35   0.729    -.4666402     .667131
         Q3  │   1.221205   .3002627     4.07   0.000     .6327005    1.809709
         Q4  │   2.141073   .3344413     6.40   0.000      1.48558    2.796565
             │
       _cons │   -1.70499   .3157896    -5.40   0.000    -2.323927   -1.086054
─────────────┴────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────

. scalar sl = e(ll)

. estimates store beyond

. di exp(_b[2.gender]) - 1
.32791427

. gen third = educg == 3

. logit third i.gender i.prestigeg i.reasong if beyond, nolog

Logistic regression                                     Number of obs =    232
                                                        LR chi2(7)    =  34.81
                                                        Prob > chi2   = 0.0000
Log likelihood = -115.21048                             Pseudo R2     = 0.1312

─────────────┬────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
       third │ Coefficient  Std. err.      z    P>|z|     [95% conf. interval]
─────────────┼────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
      gender │
     female  │  -.0541974     .33231    -0.16   0.870     -.705513    .5971182
             │
   prestigeg │
         Q2  │   .6368128   .5673881     1.12   0.262    -.4752474    1.748873
         Q3  │   .4121018   .5852127     0.70   0.481     -.734894    1.559098
         Q4  │   .8775737    .535457     1.64   0.101    -.1719027     1.92705
             │
     reasong │
         Q2  │   2.108388   1.090134     1.93   0.053    -.0282357    4.245011
         Q3  │   2.180404   1.066049     2.05   0.041     .0909867    4.269821
         Q4  │   3.327179   1.044294     3.19   0.001       1.2804    5.373958
             │
       _cons │  -4.133982   1.126399    -3.67   0.000    -6.341682   -1.926281
─────────────┴────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────

. scalar sl = sl + e(ll)

. estimates store third

. di exp(_b[2.gender]) - 1
-.05275488
> irish <- mutate(irish, beyond = educg != "junior", third = educg == "3rd level")
> sl1 <- glm(beyond ~ gender + prestigeg + reasong, data=irish, family=binomial)
> summary(sl1)

Call:
glm(formula = beyond ~ gender + prestigeg + reasong, family = binomial, 
    data = irish)

Deviance Residuals: 
    Min       1Q   Median       3Q      Max  
-2.1568  -0.9986   0.5066   0.9328   1.9349  

Coefficients:
             Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)    
(Intercept)   -1.7050     0.3158  -5.399 6.70e-08 ***
genderfemale   0.2836     0.2232   1.271 0.203803    
prestigegQ2    0.9807     0.2927   3.350 0.000808 ***
prestigegQ3    1.2687     0.3280   3.869 0.000110 ***
prestigegQ4    1.5035     0.3171   4.741 2.13e-06 ***
reasongQ2      0.1002     0.2892   0.347 0.728899    
reasongQ3      1.2212     0.3003   4.067 4.76e-05 ***
reasongQ4      2.1411     0.3344   6.402 1.53e-10 ***
---
Signif. codes:  0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1

(Dispersion parameter for binomial family taken to be 1)

    Null deviance: 601.10  on 434  degrees of freedom
Residual deviance: 500.74  on 427  degrees of freedom
AIC: 516.74

Number of Fisher Scoring iterations: 4

> sl2 <- glm(third ~ gender + prestigeg + reasong, data=filter(irish, beyond), family=binomial)
> summary(sl2)

Call:
glm(formula = third ~ gender + prestigeg + reasong, family = binomial, 
    data = filter(irish, beyond))

Deviance Residuals: 
   Min      1Q  Median      3Q     Max  
-1.208  -0.766  -0.588   1.148   2.587  

Coefficients:
             Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)    
(Intercept)   -4.1340     1.1264  -3.670 0.000242 ***
genderfemale  -0.0542     0.3323  -0.163 0.870445    
prestigegQ2    0.6368     0.5674   1.122 0.261710    
prestigegQ3    0.4121     0.5852   0.704 0.481313    
prestigegQ4    0.8776     0.5355   1.639 0.101229    
reasongQ2      2.1084     1.0901   1.934 0.053104 .  
reasongQ3      2.1804     1.0660   2.045 0.040822 *  
reasongQ4      3.3272     1.0443   3.186 0.001442 ** 
---
Signif. codes:  0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1

(Dispersion parameter for binomial family taken to be 1)

    Null deviance: 265.23  on 231  degrees of freedom
Residual deviance: 230.42  on 224  degrees of freedom
AIC: 246.42

Number of Fisher Scoring iterations: 6

(b) Interpret the coefficients for females in both equations in terms of odds or conditional odds. Is this result broadly consistent with the results of part 1?

> b <- c(coef(sl1)["genderfemale"], coef(sl2)["genderfemale"])
> exp(b) - 1
genderfemale genderfemale 
  0.32791427  -0.05275488 

The odds of going beyond the junior level are 32.8% higher for females than males in the same quartile of parental occupational prestige and reasoning score. Among those who go beyond the junior level, the (conditional) odds of achieving third level are 5.3% lower for females than males in the same category of occupational prestige and reasoning score.

The results are generally consistent with the previous model, both estimating a higher probability of going beyond the junior level, but then a lower probability of going beyond the senior level conditional on having gotten there. It is not immediately obvious whether these two effects cancel each other in terms of the probability of achieving third level, but marginal effects can clear than up.

(c) Predict the average probability of reaching 3rd level if everyone was male and then if everyone was female, and compare your results with the corresponding answer from part 1.

. forvalues g = 1/2 {
  2.   quietly replace gender = `g'
  3.   quietly estimates restore beyond
  4.   predict p`g'b, pr
  5.   quietly estimates restore third
  6.   predict p`g'tb, pr
  7.   gen p`g't = p`g'b * p`g'tb
  8. }

. quietly replace gender = keep

. gen des = p2t - p1t

. sum p2t p1t des

    Variable │        Obs        Mean    Std. dev.       Min        Max
─────────────┼─────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
         p2t │        435    .1399573    .1444416   .0029064   .4548946
         p1t │        435    .1348909    .1441322   .0024251    .452615
         des │        435    .0050663    .0036268   .0004813   .0124282
> pb_male <- predict(sl1, type = "response", newdata = males)
> pt.b_male <- predict(sl2, type = "response", newdata = males)
> pt_male <- pb_male * pt.b_male
> pb_female <- predict(sl1, type = "response", newdata = females)
> pt.b_female <- predict(sl2, type = "response", newdata = females)
> pt_female <- pb_female * pt.b_female
> c(female = mean(pt_female), male = mean(pt_male), 
+   diff = mean(pt_female) - mean(pt_male))
     female        male        diff 
0.139957258 0.134890925 0.005066332 

We find a difference of half a percentage point, not unlike the 0.34 we found in the previous model, indicating essentially no gender differences in the overall probability of reaching third level.

(d) Compare the sequential and multinomial logit models in terms of parsimony, goodness of fit, and how well they represent gender differences in educational attaintment.

. di ml, sl
-365.44159 -365.58016
> c(logLik(ml), logLik(sl1) + logLik(sl2))
[1] -365.4416 -365.5802

The models have very similar log-likelihoods, -365.4 for the multinomial logit and -365.6 for the sequential logit, with a tiny difference in favor of multinomial logits, and exactly the same number of parameters, so there really isn’t much to choose here. From what we have seen, they also lead to similar predictions by gender, so from this point of view we could declare it a tie.

Taking a closer look, the generally small gender differences are confined to the contrast between junior and senior levels, which happens to be one of the two contrasts in the multinomial logit model, so that would be a point in its favor. On the other hand, the sequential logit model shows that girls have a higher probability of going beyond junior, but then a lower conditional probability of continuing to third, which I think shows more clearly what’s going on. This would tip the balance towards sequential logits.

(e) Comment of the coefficients that correspond to children in the top quartile of the reasoning test. Do we need marginal effects to determine if they have a higher probability of reaching 3rd level than comparable children with lower scores?

In case it was not clear this question refers just to the sequential logit model. In this model, children in the top quartile of reasoning scores, compared with children in lower quartiles, with the same gender and parental occupational prestige, have a higher probability of going beyond junior level and also a higher conditional probability of continuing to third level given that they went beyond junior, so obviously the overall probability is also higher. Marginal effects are clearly not needed to settle this issue.

(If you think in terms of the multinomial logit model, the answer is not so clear. Kids in the upper quartile of reasoning have higher relative probabilities of senior compared to junior, and generally much higher relative probabilities or third compared to junior, so it looks like the overall probability of third level should be higher, but I would compute marginal effects to make sure. In fact they do.)

[3] Ordered Logits

(a) Fit an ordered logit model to the same data using the same predictors.

. ologit educg i.gender i.prestigeg i.reasong, nolog

Ordered logistic regression                             Number of obs =    435
                                                        LR chi2(7)    = 125.58
                                                        Prob > chi2   = 0.0000
Log likelihood = -370.37744                             Pseudo R2     = 0.1450

─────────────┬────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
       educg │ Coefficient  Std. err.      z    P>|z|     [95% conf. interval]
─────────────┼────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
      gender │
     female  │   .1974119   .1987321     0.99   0.321     -.192096    .5869197
             │
   prestigeg │
         Q2  │   .9903856   .2794842     3.54   0.000     .4426066    1.538165
         Q3  │   1.165486   .3021775     3.86   0.000     .5732289    1.757743
         Q4  │    1.47537   .2896877     5.09   0.000      .907592    2.043147
             │
     reasong │
         Q2  │   .2097262   .2829288     0.74   0.459     -.344804    .7642565
         Q3  │   1.238899   .2832053     4.37   0.000     .6838272    1.793971
         Q4  │   2.354729   .2989482     7.88   0.000     1.768801    2.940656
─────────────┼────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
       /cut1 │   1.693513   .3016261                      1.102337     2.28469
       /cut2 │   4.170666   .3600605                      3.464961    4.876372
─────────────┴────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────────

. scalar ol = e(ll)

. scalar odf = e(df_m)
> library(MASS)
> ol <- polr(educg ~ gender + prestigeg + reasong, data = irish)    
> summary(ol)
Call:
polr(formula = educg ~ gender + prestigeg + reasong, data = irish)

Coefficients:
              Value Std. Error t value
genderfemale 0.1974     0.1987  0.9933
prestigegQ2  0.9904     0.2795  3.5436
prestigegQ3  1.1655     0.3022  3.8570
prestigegQ4  1.4754     0.2897  5.0929
reasongQ2    0.2097     0.2829  0.7413
reasongQ3    1.2389     0.2832  4.3745
reasongQ4    2.3547     0.2989  7.8767

Intercepts:
                 Value   Std. Error t value
junior|senior     1.6935  0.3016     5.6146
senior|3rd level  4.1707  0.3601    11.5832

Residual Deviance: 740.7549 
AIC: 758.7549 

(b) Interpret the estimate of the first cutpoint in terms of odds or probabilities, keeping in mind the reference cell used in the model.

. di exp(_b[/cut1]), invlogit(_b[/cut1])
5.4385543 .84468563
> cut1 <- ol$zeta[1]
> c(exp(cut1), plogis(cut1))
junior|senior junior|senior 
    5.4385069     0.8446845 

For males in the lower quartile of parental occupational prestige and reasoning score, the odds of being in junior level are 5.44 to 1, which gives a probability of 84.5%,

(c) Interpret the coefficient of females in terms of (i) a latent variable, and (ii) the odds of progressing past junior and senior levels.

. di _b[educg:2.gender]/(_pi/sqrt(3))
.10883886

. di exp(_b[educg:2.gender])
1.2182457
> b <- coef(ol)["genderfemale"]
> c(b/(pi/sqrt(3)), exp(b))
genderfemale genderfemale 
   0.1088369    1.2182413 

In terms of a latent scale of educational achievement, girls are on average about one-tenth of a standard deviation higher than boys in the same category of parental occupational prestige and reasoning score. These girls also have 21.8% higher odds of going beyond junior level, as well as 21.8% higher odds of going beyond senior level, than boys in the same category of occupational prestige and reasoning score. (I think this is clearer than saying they have 21.8% higher odds or achieving third level rather than junior or senior, as well as 21.8% higher odds of achieving senior or third level rather than junior.)

(d) Predict the probability of reaching 3rd level if everyone was male and then if everyone was female, and compare your result with the corresponding answer from part 2.

. forvalues g = 1/2 {
  2.   quietly replace gender = `g'
  3.   predict o`g'1 o`g'2 o`g'3, pr
  4. }

. quietly replace gender = keep

. gen ode = o23 - o13

. sum o23 o13 ode

    Variable │        Obs        Mean    Std. dev.       Min        Max
─────────────┼─────────────────────────────────────────────────────────
         o23 │        435    .1505351    .1414167   .0184647    .464272
         o13 │        435    .1302775    .1265154   .0152071   .4156715
         ode │        435    .0202576     .015174   .0032576   .0486004
> pol_male   <- predict(ol, type = "probs", newdata = males) 
> pol_female <- predict(ol, type = "probs", newdata = females) 
> mo_male   <- apply(pol_male, 2, mean)
> mo_female <- apply(pol_female, 2, mean)
> data.frame(rbind(female = mo_female, male = mo_male, diff = mo_female - mo_male))
            junior    senior X3rd.level
female  0.44597704 0.4034877 0.15053524
male    0.48405429 0.3856677 0.13027799
diff   -0.03807725 0.0178200 0.02025725

We see that on average girls have a 15% probability of reaching third level, while comparable boys have an average probability of 13%, a difference of two percentage points. This is larger than we had estimated using multinomial or sequential logit models.

(e) How well does this model stack up against the previous two? Make sure you consider parsimony, goodness of fit, and how well the model reflects gender differences in educational attaintment.

. di -2 * ml + 2 * 16
762.88317

. di -2 * ol + 2 *  9
758.75489
> c(ml = AIC(ml),  ol = AIC(ol))
      ml       ol 
762.8832 758.7549 

The model is clearly more parsimonious than the previous ones, using only 9 parameters instead of 16, a savings of 7. The log-likelihood of -370.4 is 4.9 points lower than the multinomial logit model, so the fit is a bit worse. Using Akaike’s criterion, with a deviance penalty of two points per parameter, we obtain a small advantage of 4.1 points for the ordered logit model.

In terms of representing gender differences, however, the ordered logit model is not as good as the previous ones, because it assumes a uniform shift in academic achievement. This should result in girls having higher progressions from junior to senior as well as senior to third, whereas the evidence indicates that they have a somewhat higher probability of progressing to senior level but then a somewhat lower probability of continuing to third level, so in the end we see no gender differences in the probability of achieving third level.